A Straight Dope Classic from Cecil's Storehouse of Human Knowledge

How accurate are police sketches of suspected criminals?

May 7, 2010

Dear Cecil:

We recently learned a rapist was loose in our neighborhood when my roommate got an an e-mail from her school that included a link to a police sketch of the subject. This got me thinking about the accuracy of police sketches. How much should I trust the rendering we saw? Has anyone done a study comparing sketches to the actual suspects who were later apprehended?

Cecil replies:

There have been lots of studies, which I'll get to in a minute. The one thing I couldn't find was a systematic online comparison of suspect sketches and actual photos of the criminals. So I spent the afternoon rummaging around on the Web looking for examples involving well-known wrongdoers. What I came up with can be seen at mugshot-sketch-comparison. Ultimately the results confounded my expectations:

1. David Berkowitz, aka Son of Sam. This was the first set I found, and it seemed to confirm my initial biases: in my opinion, the face is too narrow, the cheekbones are too high, and the hair and eyebrows are wrong.

2. Ted Kaczynski, aka the Unabomber. The sunglasses in this famous drawing conceal so much I initially thought it could be anybody. In Kaczynski's mug shot he has a beard, but when I compared the prominent jawline with that of a younger, shaven Kaczynski, the similarity was striking enough to make the sketch seem informative.

3. Timothy McVeigh, the Oklahoma City bomber. Not a perfect likeness by any stretch; the forehead in the drawing is too broad. But the eyes, nose, and mouth give you the right general idea. The FBI claims the drawing was close enough that hotel employees who saw it were able to identify McVeigh.

4. Ted Bundy, serial killer. The face in the drawing is too narrow, but the eyes and eyebrows show a strong resemblance. Plus your overall impression is that this is a handsome fellow, which Bundy certainly was.

5. Bruno Hauptmann, the kidnapper of the Lindbergh baby. OK, the nose is a little off, but the eyes are a dead match.

Granted, I may have inadvertently cherry-picked the best cases; the McVeigh and Hauptmann illustrations came from the FBI's Web site as examples of successful IDs. Still, the upshot is that sometimes police sketches work.

More often, though, they don't. Though I'm not aware of any comprehensive survey, few doubt misses far outnumber the hits. The problems are obvious. Eyewitnesses are stressed, forgetful, and often wrong — one study of 180 convicts freed by DNA evidence found more than 80 percent were mistakenly locked up due in part to erroneous identification. And the skill of the artists varies widely; for every masterful rendering you can find half a dozen others that look like they were done by a fifth-grader.

And often there's no artist at all. Instead, investigators may use a set of standardized facial features to piece together a composite portrait with the witness's assistance. Originally these were kits containing transparencies of different mouths, eyes, ears, etc, that could be mixed and matched; now the job is typically done with computers. The choices are limited, and the cops in many cases have minimal training or experience. The results are often dismal. For example:

  • In 2004 Arizona State undergrads viewed photographs of eight British celebrities and made 48 drawings of them using pro-grade composite software. Not one of the drawings was successfully identified by a group of student volunteers in the UK. When other British students were asked to match the original photos with the composites, the accuracy rate was about 6 in 10.
  • Amateur composite makers in Britain didn't do much better. When 80 students from the University of Stirling, in Scotland, were each shown ten celebrity composites made by local volunteers — a total of 800 views — there were only 22 correct IDs, an accuracy rate of about 3 percent. When the students tried to match the composites with the original photographs, the accuracy rate was only 42 percent.

Some anecdata: My assistant Una went through the composite-making process when she was robbed at gunpoint. Although she tried her best, the result didn't look much like the suspect when he was caught. (She did identify him in a lineup, however.)

The latest development is computer software that morphs together drawings or composites produced with the help of different witnesses (or the same witness making several attempts) to generate a consensus that supposedly approximates reality more closely. In the lab it seems to work pretty well; see http://www.forensicmag.com/Assets/images/0608/art4fig1.jpg. In the real world? Not too bad, I guess. Go look at Figure 1 in the document at Frowd_et_al._(2006) and judge for yourself.

Is the whole business pointless or counterproductive? A wildly inaccurate composite arguably makes it easier for the bad guy to escape. On the other hand, police renderings do help once in a while, and if you've got a killer or rapist on the loose, what else are you supposed to do? Tell the public to keep a sharp eye out for somebody of medium height, medium complexion, and brown hair?

Related Posts with Thumbnails

References

Czerederecka, A. et al. (Eds) Forensic Psychology and Law: Facing the Challenges of a Changing World Krakow: Institute of Forensic Research Publishers, 2004.

Davies, Graham, Willik, Paul van der, and Morrison, Lisa J. “Facial Composite Production: A Comparison of Mechanical and Computer-Driven Systems” Journal of Applied Psychology 85.1 (2000): 119-124.

Frowd, C.D. et al. “An Evaluation of US Systems for Facial Composite Production” Ergonomics 50.12 (2007): 1987-1998.

Frowd, Charlie et al. “Contemporary Composite Techniques: The Impact of a Forensically-Relevant Target Delay” Legal and Criminological Psychology 10 (2005): 63-81.

Green, Debra L. and Geiselman, Edward. “Building Composite Facial Images: Effect of Feature Saliency and Delay of Construction” Journal of Applied Psychology 74.5 (1989): 714-721.

Jenkins, Felicity and Davies, Graham. “Contamination of Facial Memory Through Exposure to Misleading Composite Pictures” Journal of Applied Psychology 70.1 (1985): 164-176.

McQuiston-Surrett, Dawn et al. “Evaluation of Facial Composite Evidence Depends on the Presence of Other Case Factors” Legal and Criminological Psychology 12 (2008): 279-298.

McQuiston-Surrett, Dawn et al. “Use of Facial Composite Systems in US Law Enforcement Agencies” Psychology, Crime, & Law 12.5 (2006): 505-517.

Taylor, Karen T. Forensic Art and Illustration Boca Raton: CRC Press, 2001.

Recent Additions:

A Straight Dope Classic by Cecil Adams
A Straight Dope Staff Report by SDStaff Hawk, Straight Dope Science Advisory Board
A Straight Dope Staff Report by SDStaff Ken,
A Straight Dope Classic by Cecil Adams
A Straight Dope Staff Report by SDStaff Doug, Straight Dope Science Advisory Board
A Straight Dope Classic by Cecil Adams
A Straight Dope Staff Report by SDStaff Chronos, Straight Dope Science Advisory Board
A Straight Dope Classic by Cecil Adams
A Straight Dope Staff Report by SDStaff Doug, Straight Dope Science Advisory Board
A Straight Dope Classic by Cecil Adams
A Straight Dope Staff Report by SDStaff ScottInLA, Straight Dope Science Advisory Board

Send questions for Cecil Adams to: cecil@chicagoreader.com

Send comments about this website to: webmaster@straightdope.com

Terms of Use / Privacy Policy

Advertise on the Straight Dope! Your direct line to thou- sands of the smartest, hippest people on the planet, plus a few total dipsticks.

Publishers - interested in subscribing to the Straight Dope? Write to: sdsubscriptions@chicagoreader.com.

Copyright © 2017 Sun-Times Media, LLC.